|
The Theory Underlying Concept Maps and How To Construct Them
Joseph D. Novak, Cornell University
C oncept maps are
tools for organizing and representing knowledge. They include concepts,
usually enclosed in circles or boxes of some type, and relationships
between concepts or propositions, indicated by a connecting line between
two concepts. Words on the line specify the relationship between the two
concepts. We define concept as a perceived regularity in events or
objects, or records of events or objects, designated by a label. The
label for most concepts is a word, although sometimes we use symbols such
as + or %. Propositions are statements about some object or event in
the universe, either naturally occurring or constructed. Propositions
contain two or more concepts connected with other words to form a
meaningful statement. Sometimes these are called semantic units,or
units of meaning. Figure
1 shows an example of a concept map that describes the structure of
concept maps and illustrates the above characteristics.
 click for larger image
Figure 1
Another characteristic of concept maps is that the concepts are
represented in a hierarchical fashion with the most inclusive, most
general concepts at the top of the map and the more specific, less general
concepts arranged hierarchically below. The hierarchical structure for a
particular domain of knowledge also depends on the context in which that
knowledge is being applied or considered. Therefore, it is best to
construct concept maps with reference to some particular question we seek
to answer or some situation or event that we are trying to understand
through the organization of knowledge in the form of a concept map.
Another important characteristic of concept maps is the inclusion of
"cross-links." These are relationships (propositions) between concepts in
different domains of the concept map. Cross-links help us to see how some
domains of knowledge represented on the map are related to each
other.Inthe creation of new knowledge, cross-links often represent
creative leaps on the part of the knowledge producer. There are two
features of concept maps that are important in the facilitation of
creative thinking: the hierarchical structure that is represented in a
good map and the ability to search for and characterize cross-links.A
final features that may be added to concept maps are specific examples of
events or objects that help to clarify the meaning of a given concept.
Concept maps were developed in the course of our research program where
we sought to follow and understand changes in childrenšs know ledge of
science. This program was based on the learning psychology of David
Ausubel (1963, 1968, 1978). The fundamental idea in Ausubel's cognitive
psychology is that learning takes place by the assimilation of new
concepts and propositions into existing concept propositional frameworks
held by the learner. The question sometimes arises as to the origin of the
first concepts; these are acquired by children during the ages of birth to
three years, when they recognize regularities in the world around them and
begin to identify language labels or symbols for these regularities
(Macnamara, 1982). This is a phenomenal ability that is part of the
evolutionary heritage of all normal human beings. After age 3, new concept
and propositional learning is mediated heavily by language, and takes
place primarily by a reception learning process where new meanings
are obtained by asking questions and getting clarification of
relationships between old concepts and propositions and new concepts and
propositions. This acquisition is mediated in a very important way when
concrete experiences or props are available; hence the importance of
"hands-on" activity for science learning with young children, but this is
also true with learners of any age and in any subject matter domain. In
addition to the distinction between the discovery learning process, where
the attributes of concepts are identified autonomously by the learner, and
the reception learning process, where attributes of concepts are described
using language and transmitted to the learner, Ausubel made the very
important distinction between rote learning and meaningful learning.
Meaningful learning requires three conditions:
- The material to be learned must be conceptually clear and presented
with language and examples relatable to the learner's prior knowledge.
Concept maps can be helpful to meet this condition, both by identifying
large general concepts prior to instruction in more specific concepts,
and by assisting in the sequencing of learning tasks though
progressively more explicit knowledge that can be anchored into
developing conceptual frameworks.
- The learner must possess relevant prior knowledge. This condition is
easily met after age 3 for virtually any domain of subject matter, but
it is necessary to be careful and explicit in building concept
frameworks if one hopes to present detailed specific knowledge in any
field in subsequent lessons. We see, therefore, that conditions (1) and
(2) are interrelated and both are important.
- The learner must choose to learn meaningfully. The one condition
over which the teacher or mentor has only indirect control is the
motivation of students to choose to learn by attempting to incorporate
new meanings into their prior knowledge, rather than simply memorizing
concept definitions or propositional statements or computational
procedures. The control over this choice is primarily in the evaluation
strategies used, and typical objective tests seldom require more than
rote learning (Holden, 1992). In fact, the worst forms of objective
tests, or short-answers tests, require verbatim recall of statements and
this may be impeded by meaningful learning where new knowledge is
assimilated into existing frameworks, making it difficult to recall
specific, verbatim definitions or descriptions. This kind of problem was
recognized years ago in Hoffman's (1962), The Tyranny of Testing.
One of the powerful uses of concept maps is not only as a learning tool
but also as an evaluation tool, thus encouraging students to use
meaningful-mode learning patterns (Novak & Gowin, 1984; Novak, 1990,
Mintzes, Wandersee and Novak, 2000). Concept maps are also effective in
identifying both valid and invalid ideas held by students. They can be as
effective as more time-consuming clinical interviews (Edwards &
Fraser, 1983).
Another important advance in our understanding of learning is that the
human memory is not a single "vessel" to be filled, but rather a complex
set of interrelated memory systems. Figure 2 illustrates
the three memory systems of the human mind.
 click for larger image
Figure 2
While all memory systems are interdependent (and have information going
in both directions), the most critical memory system for incorporating
knowledge into long-term memory is the short-term or "working memory." All
incoming information is organized and processed in the working memory by
interaction with knowledge in long-term memory. The limiting feature here
is that working memory can process only a relatively small number (five to
nine) of psychological units at any one moment. This means that
relationships among two or three concepts are about the limit of working
memory processing capacity. Therefore, to structure large bodies of
knowledge requires an orderly sequence of iterations between working
memory and long-term memory as new knowledge is being received (Anderson,
1991). We believe one of the reasons concept mapping is so powerful for
the facilitation of meaningful learning is that it serves as a kind of
template to help to organize knowledge and to structure it, even though
the structure must be built up piece by piece with small units of
interacting concept and propositional frameworks. Many learners and
teachers are surprised to see how this simple tool facilitates meaningful
learning and the creation of powerful knowledge frameworks that not only
permit utilization of the knowledge in new contexts, but also retention of
the knowledge for long periods of time (Novak, 1990; Novak &
Wandersee, 1991). There is still relatively little known about memory
processes and how knowledge finally gets incorporated into our brain, but
it seems evident from diverse sources of research that our brain works to
organize knowledge in hierarchical frameworks and that learning approaches
that facilitate this process significantly enhance the learning capability
of all learners.
While it is true that some students have more difficulty building
concept maps and using these, at least early in their experience, this
appears to result primarily from years of rote-mode learning practice in
school settings rather than as a result of brain structure differences per
se. Socalled "learning style" differences are, to a large extent,
differences in the patterns of learning that students have employed
varying from high commitment to continuous rote-mode learning to almost
exclusive commitment to meaningful mode learning. It is not easy to help
students in the former condition move to patterns of learning of the
latter type. While concept maps can help, students also need to be taught
something about brain mechanisms and knowledge organization,and this
instruction should accompany the use of concept maps.
Epistemological Foundations
As indicated earlier, we defined concepts as
perceived regularities in events or objects, or records of events or
objects, designated by labels. What is coming to be generally recognized
now is that the meaningful learning processes described above are the same
processes used by scientists and mathematicians to construct new
knowledge. In fact, I have argued that knowledge construction is nothing
other than a relatively high level of meaningful learning (Novak, 1977;
Novak, 1988).
As defined above, conepts and propositons are the building blocks for
knowledge in any domain. We can use the analogy that concepts are like the
atoms of matter and propositions are like the molecules of matter. There
are now about 460,000 words in the English language, and these can be
comibined to form an infinite number of propositions; albeit most
combinations of words might be nonsense, there is still the possibility of
creating an infinite number of valid propositions. We shall never run out
of opportunities to create new knowledge! As people create and observe new
or exisiting objects or events, the creative people will continue to
create new knowledge.
While there is value in studying more extensively with the process of
knowledge construction, and the nature of knowledge, this is beyond the
scope of this document.
Constructing Good Concept Maps
In learning to construct a concept map, it is
important to begin with a domain of knowledge that is very familiar to the
person constructing the map. Since concept map structures are dependent on
the context in which they will be used, it is best to identify a segment
of a text, a laboratory activity, or a particular problem or question that
one is trying to understand. This creates a context that will help to
determine the hierarchical structure of the concept map. It is also
helpful to select a limited domain of knowledge for the first concept
maps.
Once a domain has been selected, the next step is to identify the key
concepts that apply to this domain. These could be listed, and then from
this list a rank order should be established from the most general, most
inclusive concept, for this particular problem or situation, to the most
specific, least general concept. Although this rank order may be only
approximate, it helps to begin the process of map construction.
The next step is to construct a preliminary concept map. This can be
done by writing all of the concepts on Post-its, or preferably by using
this computer software program. Post-its allow a group to work on a
whiteboard or butcher paper and to move concepts around easily This is
necessary as one begins to struggle with the process of building a good
hierarchical organization. Computer software programs are even better in
that they allow moving of concepts together with linking statements and
also the moving of groups of concepts and links to restructure the map.
They also permit a computer printout, producing a nice product that can be
e-mailed or in other ways easily shared with collaborators or pother
interested parties.
Figure 3 shows
a list of concepts for making a concept map to address the question, "What
is a plant?" What is shown is only one of many possible maps. Simple as
this map is, it may contain some propositions that are new to the reader.
It is important to recognize that a concept map is never finished.
After a preliminary map is constructed, it is always necessary to revise
this map. Good maps usually undergo three to many revisions. This is one
reason why computer software is helpful.
After a preliminary map is constructed, cross-links should be sought.
These are links between different domains of knowledge on the map that
help to illustrate how these domains are related to one another. Finally,
the map should be revised, concepts positioned in ways that lend to
clarity, and a "final" map prepared.
When computer software is used, one can go back and change the size and
font style to "dress up" the concept map.
 click for larger image
Figure 3
It is important to help students recognize that all concepts are in
some way related to one another. Therefore, it is necessary to be
selective in identifying cross-links, and to be as precise as possible in
identifying linking words that connect concepts. In addition, one should
avoid "sentences in the boxes" since this usually indicates that a whole
subsection of the map could be constructed from the statement in the box.
"String maps" illustrate either poor understanding of the material or an
inadequate restructuring of the map. Figure 4 shows an
example of a string map.
 click for larger image
Figure 4
Students often comment that it is hard to add linking words onto their
concept map. This is because they only poorly understand the relationship
between the concepts and it is the linking words that specify this
relationship. Once students begin to focus in on good linking words, and
also identification of good cross-links, they can see that every concept
could be related to every other concept. This also produces some
frustration, and they must choose to identify the most prominent and most
useful cross-links. This process involves what Bloom (1956) identified as
high levels of cognitive performance, namely evaluation and synthesis of
knowledge. Concept mapping is an easy way to achieve very high levels of
cognitive performance, when the process is done well. This is one reason
concept mapping can be a very powerful evaluation tool.
Macro and Micro Concept Maps
In curriculum planning, concept maps can be
enormously useful. They present in a highly concise manner the key
concepts and principles to be taught. The hierarchical organization of
concept maps suggests more optimal sequencing of instructional material.
Since the fundamental characteristic of meaningful learning is integration
of new knowledge with the learners' previous concept and propositional
frameworks, proceeding from the more general, more inclusive concepts to
the more specific information usually serves to encourage and enhance
meaningful learning. Thus, in curriculum planning, we need to construct a
global "macro map" showing the major ideas we plan to present in the whole
course, or in a whole curriculum, and also more specific "micro maps" to
show the knowledge structure for a very specific segment of the
instructional program. Figure 5 shows a
macro map constructed for this CD-ROM and gives a "global view" of all the
content of the CD. Figure 6 shows a "micro
map" map expanding on ideas for which the concept "Human Exploration" is
shown in the "macro map". In turn, one could click on the map icons in figure 6 and see
further detailed knowledge about MARS.
 click for larger image
Figure 5
Using concept maps in planning a curriculum or instruction on a
specific topic helps to make the instruction "conceptually transparent" to
students. Many students have difficulty identifying and constructing
powerful concept and propositional frameworks, leading them to see science
learning as a blur of myriad facts or equations to be memorized. If
concept maps are used in planning instruction and students are required to
construct concept maps as they are learning, previously unsuccessful
students can become successful in making sense out of science and
acquiring a feeling of control over the subject matter (Bascones &
Novak, 1985; Novak, 1991; Novak, 1998).
 click for larger image
Figure 6
Facilitating Cooperative Learning
There is a growing body of research that shows
that when students work in small groups and cooperate in striving to learn
subject matter, positive cognitive and affective outcomes result (Johnson
et al., 1981). In our work with both teachers and students, small groups
working cooperatively to construct concept maps have proven to be useful
in many contexts. For example, the concept maps shown in Figure 7 was
constructed by faculty working together to plan instruction in veterinary
medicine at Cornell University. In my own classes, and in classes taught
by my students, small groups of students working collectively to construct
concept maps can produce some remarkably good maps. In a variety of
educational settings, concept mapping in small groups has served us well
in tasks as diverse as understanding ideas in assimilation theory to
clarifying job conflicts for conflict resolution in profit and non-profit
corporations. Concept maps are now beginning to be used in corporations to
help teams clarify and articulate the knowledge needed to solve problems
ranging from the design of new products to marketing to administrative
problem resolution.
 click for larger
image Figure 7
Concept Maps for Evaluation
We are now beginning to see in many science
textbooks the inclusion of concept mapping as one way to summarize
understandings acquired by students after they study a unit or chapter.
Change in school practices is always slow, but it is likely that the use
of concept maps in school instruction will increase substantially in the
next decade or two. When concept maps are used in instruction, they can
also be used for evaluation. There is nothing written in stone that says
multiple choice tests must be used from grade school through university,
and perhaps in time even national achievement exams will utilize concept
mapping as a powerful evaluation tool. This is a chicken-and-egg problem
because concept maps cannot be required on national achievement tests, if
most students have not been given opportunities to learn to use this
knowledge representation tool. On the other hand, if state, regional, and
national exams would begin to include concept maps as a segment of the
exam, there would be a great incentive for teachers to teach students how
to use this tool. Hopefully, by the year 2061, this will come to pass.
References
- Anderson, O. R. (1992). Some interrelationships between
constructivist models of learning and current neurobiological theory,
with implications for science education. Journal of Research in
Science Teaching, 29(10), 1037-1058.
- Ausubel, D. P. (1963). The Psychology of Meaningful Verbal
Learning. New York: Grune and Stratton.
- Ausubel, D. P. (1968). Educational Psychology: A Cognitive
View. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
- Ausubel, D. P., J. D. Novak, and H. Hanesian. (1978). Educational
Psychology: A Cognitive View, 2nd ed. New York: Holt, Rinehart and
Winston. Reprinted, New York: Warbel & Peck, 1986.
- Bascones, J., & J. D. Novak. (1985). Alternative instructional
systems and the development of problem-solving skills in physics.
European Journal of Science Education, 7(3), 253-261.
- Bloom, B. S. (1956). Taxonomy of Educational Objectives--The
Classification of Educational Goals. New York: David McKay.
- Edwards, J., and K. Fraser. (1983). Concept maps as reflectors of
conceptual understanding. Research in Science Education, 13,
19-26.
- Hoffman, B. (1962). The Tyranny of Testing. New York:
Corwell-Collier.
- Holden, C. (1992). Study flunks science and math tests.
Science, 26, 541.
- Johnson, D., G. Maruyama, R. Johnson, D. Nelson, and L. Skon.
(1981). The effects of cooperative, competitive and individualistic goal
structure on achievement: A meta-analysis. Psychological
Bulletin, 89, 47-62.
- Macnamara, J. (1982). Names for Things: A Study of Human
Learning. Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press.
- Mintzes, J., Wandersee, J. and Novak, J. (1998) Teaching Science
For Understanding. San Diego: Academic Press.
- Mintzes, J., Wandersee, J. and Novak, J. (2000) Assessing Science
Understanding. San Diego: Academic Press
- Novak, J. D. (1977). A Theory of Education. Ithaca, NY:
Cornell University Press.
- Novak, J. D. (1990). Concept maps and Vee diagrams: Two
metacognitive tools for science and mathematics education.
Instructional Science, 19, 29-52.
- Novak, J. D. (1991). Clarify with concept maps. The Science
Teacher, 58(7):45-49.
- Novak, J. D., & D. B. Gowin. (1984). Learning How to
Learn. New York and Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
- Novak, J. D., & D. Musonda. (1991). A twelve-year longitudinal
study of science concept learning. American Educational Research
Journal, 28(1), 117-153.
- Novak, J. D., & J. Wandersee, 1991. Coeditors, Special Issue on
Concept Mapping of Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 28,
10.
|